Forget 100 years of solitude鈥15 minutes may be enough to hit the mental reset button.
That鈥檚 according to a team of Rochester researchers who looked at solitude as a means of self-regulating emotions. Published in the the study finds that being alone decreases a person鈥檚 strong positive and negative emotions, and instead induces calm.
Lead researcher , a Rochester doctoral candidate in clinical and social sciences in psychology, together with veteran psychology professors and , concluded that solitude can lead to relaxation and stress reduction鈥攁s long as people actively chose to be alone. In general, solitude, the team found, has a lessening effect not only on arousing positive emotions but also strong negative emotions, ultimately leaving people calmer, more relaxed, less angry, and anxious. Professors Ryan and Deci are聽the co-founders of
Previously, the researchers note, alone time has been correlated with social rejection, withdrawal and isolation, shyness, and loneliness. Not so fast, says the Rochester trio.
鈥淪olitude can be valuable and useful at times, particularly when we want to switch off for a few moments,鈥 says Nguyen. It鈥檚 not a simple matter of good or bad when it comes to the emotional effect of solitude, the researchers write. Instead, they observed that solitude changes the 鈥渋ntensity of our inner experience, both positive and negative.鈥 Alone time accentuates low-key emotions, while dialing down stronger feelings.
Growing up in Vietnam, Nguyen says she enjoyed being alone as a child and teenager. Later, her curiosity was piqued when she realized that solitude was generally characterized as either simply good or bad鈥攚ith a lot of literature pointing to its negative effects. 鈥淚 decided to take a step back and just simply look at what solitude does, observe its effect, and let it speak for itself,鈥 Nguyen explains as the starting point of her investigations.
鈥淭hat’s when I found that solitude decreases both positive and negative affect when I only measured the arousing kind of emotions,鈥 she explains. 鈥淚 added other low-key emotions in there to see what happens and that’s when we found the deactivation effect.鈥
In a nutshell, for the deactivating, calming effect to take place a person needs to be alone without any social interactions and without electronic devices. The same calming effect happened when a person was alone and engaged in a quiet activity such as reading.
The researchers define solitude as 鈥渁 psychological experience of being alone without communications, stimuli, activities, or devices that might facilitate virtual communications such as text messaging or social media.鈥
Over the course of four experiments, the collaborators first compared solitude (simply being alone without any stimuli) to people鈥檚 experiences in social interactions and found that being alone was more calming. Second, they compared solitude to being alone with a quiet activity, such as reading and found that that, too, was calming. Third, they compared solitude to the internal stimuli of thinking certain types of thoughts. They found that if participants had positive thoughts, their positive affect was not decreased. In a fourth experiment, the Rochester researchers used a switching-replications experiment to examine a person鈥檚 active motivation for being alone.
In the three laboratory experiments, study participants sat alone for a quarter of an hour in a comfortable chair, without devices and without any social interaction, assessing their emotions on rating scales before and afterward. The researchers inquired about intense positive emotions (feeling enthusiastic or excited) and intense negative emotions (feeling angry or anxious), as well as low-intensity positive emotions (feeling calm, relaxed) and low-intensity negative emotions (feeling lonely or sad).
In the third study, the team examined the effects of making choices about one鈥檚 solitary experience by allowing some of the participants to choose what to think about as they sat by themselves, while others were assigned what to think. Those who chose what to think about had higher positive emotions, such as excitement. Conversely, those study subjects who were told to think neutral thoughts exhibited lowered positive emotions.
Clearly, autonomy in solitude鈥攂eing able to choose what to think about or actively choosing to spend time alone鈥攏ot only buffered against the negative effects of solitude on emotions, but also improved the beneficial effect of solitude on a person鈥檚 relaxation and stress levels.
鈥淲hen people willingly spend time alone, they reap the greater benefits鈥攕omething that, of course, has always been a part of ancient wisdom and practices,鈥 says Ryan. 鈥淗ere we simply demonstrate the emotional changes that account for these benefits.鈥
This finding is directly based on the autonomous motivation principle of self-determination theory, pioneered by Deci and Ryan, which holds that when individuals perceive greater autonomy in their behavioral choices, the activities yield better outcomes.
In short鈥攖hose who sat alone for 15 minutes and chose what to think about, or had positive thoughts experienced the positive effects of solitude: feeling calmer, less angry or anxious, without feeling lonely or sad. They also did not lose any of their feelings of enthusiasm or excitement.
So, why not turn off your cell phone, laptop, or tablet today and hide somewhere alone (with or without a book). Just for a quarter of an hour. Feeling your inner 鈥渙m鈥 already?